How Many?
When was the last time you counted something? What did you count? Why did you count it?
"How many?" It’s a simple question, but a complex set of learned skills goes into answering it. We’re able to quickly recognize small numbers of objects like 1, 2, or 3 dots, but for larger amounts, counting strategies come into play.
How many dots do you see?
How did you determine the number of dots?
Maybe you used an entirely different strategy. Counting is the first mathematical skill we learn, and over time we develop different strategies to count more efficiently.
Names for Number
Before we can count a set of objects, we have to learn the words for numbers. Much like learning the names of letters in the alphabet before knowing their sounds, young children learn and practice number names before knowing the quantity they represent.
One-to-One Correspondence
After memorizing number names we learn to connect them to a sense of quantity, giving us our first mathematical tool, counting one by one.
One-to-one correspondence is a skill that involves pairing a number name with each object in a set once and only once, identifying each object’s number in the set as it is counted. Practicing this one-to-one counting by physically moving or touching each object as it’s counted helps young learners begin to develop their sense of number.
Cardinality
As we continue to count numbers of objects, something else begins to develop—cardinality. Cardinality refers to the total number of objects in a group.
The last number we say when counting matches the last item in our set and is the total number of items being counted; it’s the cardinal number.
Ordinality
As our number sense develops, we learn that numbers have order, and that each number we count is one more than the previous number.
We begin to identify numbers by their order and position in a sequence of numbers. These numbers—first, second, third, and so on—are called ordinal numbers.
This counter tracks the number of cyclists who cross the bridge each day.
Thirty-three—the total cyclists that have crossed the bridge so far this day—is the cardinal number. However, each of the cyclists crossed the bridge at different times. These positions, such as the 20th cyclist, can be identified with ordinal numbers. How are cardinal and ordinal numbers related? Hint: when the 20th cyclist crossed the bridge, what was the cardinal number? What is the ordinal number for the cardinal number in this set?
Numerals
In early childhood we also learn the symbols for numbers, like the symbols "3" and "8" for the numbers “three” and “eight.”
These symbols are called numerals, and they enable us to efficiently identify any number in our number system. This is an important foundational skill needed for mathematical work.
Number Paths
By labeling each successive dot with its numeral, we create a number path, a basic visual model that helps us see numbers in sequence, growing by “one” with each dot.
In this number path, what is the number that is 2 more than 5?
What About Zero?
What’s 1 less than 1 in this number path?
How do we show zero dots? Do we add a dot to the left and put the numeral "0" on it?
Because zero has no quantity, it’s not considered to be a counting number. Zero represents an empty set where there is nothing to count.
The next time you count something, think about the counting skills you use. Notice how your math brain uses subitizing, one-to-one correspondence, cardinality, and words and numerals to identify, represent, and communicate quantity.
Simple counting skills provide the foundation for counting much larger quantities, like the number of pennies in a piggy bank. Is there a more efficient strategy for counting large groups of pennies than counting them one by one? In the next chapter, we’ll explore ways to count large quantities by grouping and arranging objects.
Learn More
How Children Learn about Numbers: A Conversation with Kelly Mix
The Early Math Collaborative from the Erikson Institute digs into 4 cognitive processes related to early counting skills with developmental psychologist Kelly Mix.
The Progression of Early Number & Counting
Graham Fletcher reviews the skills young children are born with and develop in their early math education.