Leg one banner
Chapter 1

Quantity Without Counting

A Natural Sense of Number

What do you see?

Two cars facing each other on a street
Many cars in traffic on a highway.

We frequently use numbers or quantity to describe the world around us. When we do, we’re using the math part of our brain.

How does it work? Do we count? Do we guess? Or do we just know? 

Recognizing Exact Quantities

See if you can determine the number of dots as they appear below. Press “Show” when ready. You’ll only have a second to identify the number in each set.

What was the largest set of dots you identified?

Usually, the most dots people recognize is between 5 and 7. Brain researchers have discovered that even infants can identify 1 to 3 objects, well before they know number names. 

How do we know what babies understand about numbers?

Math educators call this ability to recognize small quantities at a glance, subitizing. Humans of all ages recognize sets of 1, 2, or 3 objects without counting or even thinking about it. Some people can easily recognize sets of 4 to 7 objects.

Can you recognize the number of dots on dice without counting? 

two dice

Comparing Quantities

Researchers have also discovered that we're born with a basic ability to compare quantities. Try out your comparison skills. Press the “Show” button below and compare the number of red versus black dots. Which color has more dots? Look quickly. You’ll only have a second to decide.

Were you able to tell which color group had more dots? Were you still counting when time ran out? When was it easy to recognize the larger set of dots and when was it challenging?

It’s easy to compare groups of objects when they're small in number, like groups of 2 & 3 or 3 & 5.

A group of 2 dots is less than 3 dots
A group of 5 dots is greater than 3 dots

But quickly comparing larger groups can be challenging—when they're close in size, like 8 & 9 or even 24 & 27.

A group of 8 dots is less than 9 dots.
A group of 24 dots is less than 27.

Yet when larger groups differ greatly in size, like 18 & 10 or 24 & 40, it's easier to compare them without counting.

A group of 18 dots is greater than 9 dots.
A group of 24 dots is less than 40 dots.

Thinking about this from an evolutionary viewpoint, our natural ability to compare the size of groups can be quite helpful.

Would you rather ride in the subway car on the left or the right?

A subway car with more people.
A subway car with less people.

When making important decisions, it’s not surprising that animals—from fish to rodents to primates—also have this ability to compare quantities. For example, experiments have shown that social fish, like guppies, will naturally join the larger of two groups when one is at least twice as big as the other.

A guppy in a fish tank with a smaller group of guppies on the left side and a larger group of guppies on the right side.

This natural number sense of knowing small amounts (like with dice) and comparing numbers of objects (like a few trees versus a forest)—without counting—are the foundations of our math abilities. From this starting point, we begin our individual journey as mathematicians. In the next chapter we take our first step, learning to count.

Learn More

The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics

Cognitive neuroscientist Stanislas Dehaene’s book explores the structure of our mathematical minds.

Innate Numbers?

In this 25 minute Radiolab episode, the show hosts interview Dehaene about what infants know about numbers.

Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies

This academic paper summarizes past research on animal numerosity and introduces new research on guppies’ sense of number.